Xuesong
Li
ab,
Pan
Zeng
a,
Feilong
Wang
a,
Dai
Zhang
ab,
Yi
Zhou
c,
Rongqing
Liang
a,
Qiongrong
Ou
a,
Xiang
Wu
c and
Shuyu
Zhang
*a
aInstitute for Electric Light Sources, Department of Light Sources and Illuminating Engineering, School of Information Science and Technology, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, P. R. China. E-mail: zhangshuyu@fudan.edu.cn
bInstitute of Future Lighting, Academy for Engineering and Technology, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, P. R. China
cKey Laboratory of Micro and Nano Photonic Structures (Ministry of Education), Department of Optical Science and Engineering, School of Information Science and Technology, Shanghai Engineering Research Centre of Ultra Precision Optical Manufacturing, Fudan University, Shanghai 200433, P.R. China
First published on 8th July 2021
At present, mainstream neuromorphic hardware is based on artificial synapses; however, an engram, instead of a synapse, has recently been confirmed as the basic unit of memory, which verifies the engram theory proposed by Richard Semon in 1904. Here, we demonstrate an artificial engram device based on a nanoimprinted curable resin. The variation in the relative diffraction efficiency based on the asymmetric reversible topological change of the nanoimprinted resin enables the device to meet all the requirements for artificial engrams, including synaptic plasticity, long memory storage time, asymmetric memorizing–forgetting behaviour and measurable changes and responses. On this basis, we demonstrate the concept of realizing memory formation, memory manipulation and implantation, and memory consolidation using our artificial engram device in comparison with its biological counterpart.
New conceptsThe corner stone of current non-von Neumann architecture-based neuromorphic hardware is artificial synapse, however, proposed by Richard Semon in 1904 and experimentally verified by Tonegawa's research team in 2012, the engram theory points out an engram, instead of a synapse, is the basic unit of memory. So, the question is “Can an artificial engram be realized?”, which has not been addressed so far. Here we develop an artificial engram device based on a nanoimprinted curable resin, which meets all the requirements for engrams including synaptic plasticity, long memory storage time, asymmetric memorizing–forgetting behaviour and measurable changes and responses. Using this device, we demonstrate the concept of realizing memory formation, memory manipulation and implantation, and memory consolidation. We believe the development of artificial engram devices may open a new direction parallel to the current study of artificial synapses, and it will be interesting to see how this sort of devices work synergistically with artificial synaptic devices to promote the realization of high-efficiency brain-like computing and storage in the future. Also, the use of curable resin in this work may initiate the development of materials alike including phase change materials, photoswitch materials and hydrogels in the field of artificial engrams. |
Fascinatingly, recent studies have confirmed Richard Semon's engram theory proposed in 1904,16 which highlights that an engram, instead of a synapse, is the basic unit of memory. These findings suggest that the linked engram cell ensembles distributed over various brain regions, namely, the so-called engram complex, support the overall process of memory encoding, storage and retrieval.17–20 In 2012, Tonegawa et al. identified engram cells and demonstrated specific memory recall by activating specific memory engram cells.21 Interestingly, these cells, which serve as an index of memory content, can be artificially updated with tempered information to create a false memory.22,23 Although synaptic plasticity contributes to the formation of engrams,24 memory consolidation is not determined by synaptic plasticity in single engram cells but rather the connectivity pattern of engram cells established during encoding, the complexity of which increases as the brain anatomy evolves,25,26 challenging the widely circulated conceptions of memory consolidation. The discovery of silent engrams further suggests that there are multiple states of an engram, depending on their retrievability.24,27
With ongoing comprehensive investigations of engrams in the field of brain science, the engram has been found to be the basis and key of memory coding, storage and retrieval. Therefore, we aim to answer the following question: can we learn from the functional correlation between engrams and dynamic memories and develop artificial engrams emulating biological functions, especially in the form of a highly integrated and flexible optoelectronic device?
Fig. 1(a) shows a schematic diagram of our artificial engram device, which is a one-dimensional nanoimprinted submicrometre grating composed of a SPC-347 UVCR (FOSPIA EFiRON, Ansan, Korea), a photoresistor for UV nanoimprinting. The photograph in Fig. 1(a) shows the self-supported flexible membrane of the nanoimprinted SPC-347 UVCR. The grating period of the device was 410 nm with a duty cycle of 50% (as seen in the top view image from scanning electron microscopy (SEM)) and a groove depth of 134 nm. The process of device fabrication is illustrated in Fig. 1(b). A small amount of SPC-347 UVCR was drop-cast on both a 25 mm × 25 mm silicon master grating and a 25 mm × 25 mm clean glass slide. The silicon master grating had a grating period of 410 nm, a duty cycle of 50% and a groove depth of 150 nm, and an anti-sticking layer (trichlorosilane, Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, USA) was deposited on its surface before nanoimprinting. The master grating and glass slide were then held together face-to-face for 2 minutes to allow the SPC-347 UVCR to spread via the capillary effect and fill in all the air gaps between the grating and slide. A UV lamp was applied for 2 minutes to cure the SPC-347 UVCR. The glass slide was then separated from the silicon master grating using a blade, and the nanoimprinted SPC-347 UVCR was left on the glass slide. The nanoimprinted SPC-347 UVCR was then detached from the glass slide using a blade or ethanol and formed a nanoimprinted flexible membrane. The UV nanoimprint lithography applied here is a mature technology that offers rapid and reliable nanopattern duplication with upscaling compatibility (mass production can be realized with the aid of step-and-flash imprint lithography and roll-to-roll printing).28–30
Our artificial engram device can respond to both heating and UV irradiation but in very different ways. The memorizing process can be triggered by heating, which leads to a collapse of the grating ridges, as shown in the side view SEM image in Fig. 1(c). For instance, the groove depth decreased from 134 nm to 68 nm after a heating treatment of 120 °C for 30 seconds. This degree of topological change is heating-strength dependent, and this process can finish within several seconds. The heating process in our experiment was carried out using a hotplate, but in theory, heating can also be performed using an infrared beam to selectively choose a target heating area. The forgetting process can be carried out by natural restoration, during which the grating ridges are recovered. As shown in Fig. 1(c), the groove depth recovered to 109 nm after natural restoration for 2 days. Clearly, this process takes a much longer time than the memorizing process, therefore leading to an asymmetric memorizing–forgetting behaviour. Photographs of the SPC-347 UVCR grating after the heating and natural restoration processes as well as the pristine sample are shown in Fig. 1(c), and the disappearance and reappearance of the diffracted emission of ambient light reflects the topological change in the grating surface. Apart from the natural restoration-based forgetting process, which is regarded as a passive forgetting process, an active forgetting process can be triggered by UV irradiation. UV irradiation at a dose of approximately 100 mW cm−2 from a UV lamp can recover the grating ridges to the same extent as natural restoration but within a much shorter time length (∼3 hours). This active forgetting process plays an important role in imitating the valence switch of biological memory (described later in detail). This asymmetric memorizing–forgetting behaviour, the self-restorable capability without external stimuli and the active forgetting capability make the device a promising candidate for artificial engrams and also indicates that phase change materials,15,31–34 photoswitch materials35 and hydrogels36,37 may be able to realize more functions when mimicking neural networks than what has been achieved at present. For example, a recent study demonstrated a soft thermal swelling hydrogel with dynamic memorizing–forgetting behaviour.36
To quantify the degrees of the memorizing and forgetting processes, we measured the relative diffraction efficiencies and correlated the degrees of memorizing and forgetting with the change in efficiencies. As shown in Fig. 1(d), the device was vertically mounted on a rotational stage with the grating grooves aligned perpendicularly. A continuous-wave solid-state laser with a wavelength of 532 nm was used, and the laser emission was incident on the device at an angle of 25° to the surface normal of the device. A photodetector (PDA100A, THORLABS) was placed at an angle of 60° (-1st order) to the surface normal to measure the intensity of the diffracted emission. The diffraction efficiency of the intact device was normalized to 1, and the relative diffraction efficiencies of the stimulated devices were recorded.
Fig. 1(e–g) show the relative diffraction efficiency curves of our artificial engram device after heating, natural restoration and UV irradiation, respectively. When the device was heated at a temperature of 120 °C for 30 seconds, the relative diffraction efficiency showed a substantial decrease from 100% to 7 ± 1%. Approximately 50 hours under natural restoration conditions were required to recover the ridges, but the maximum efficiency of achievable recovery was approximately 47 ± 1%, as expected from the SEM images, which corresponds to the “silent” state of an engram.24 UV irradiation can accelerate the recovery and reach the same recovered efficiency in approximately 3 hours. The recovery effect of natural restoration and UV irradiation is the same, and the only difference is the length of time required. To confirm the device uniformity, we measured the relative diffraction efficiencies at different locations after heating and UV irradiation. The values are almost identical under the same stimulus, as shown in Fig. S1 (ESI†). To confirm the rationality of the efficiency values, we also carried out a numerical simulation of the change in diffraction efficiency based on the SEM results. The calculated efficiency values, which are in good agreement with the experimental results, are shown in Fig. S2 (ESI†).
The above-mentioned intriguing characteristics originate from the intrinsic material properties of the SPC-347 UVCR. Fig. 2(a) shows the viscoelastic properties of the UV-cured SPC-347 UVCR using dynamic mechanical analysis. Clearly, room temperature (∼20 °C) is in the glassy transition region and the operating temperature of 120 °C is in the rubbery plateau region. The large width of the rubbery plateau is due to the molecular cross-linking of the UV-cured SPC-347 UVCR. When the grating structure was fabricated at room temperature, structural stress also formed. When the temperature surpassed the dividing point of the two regions (70–80 °C), the UV-cured SPC-347 UVCR became highly elastic and thus released structural stress by increasing its entropy,38,39 leading to a morphological change in the grating. When the SPC-347 UVCR was cooled to room temperature, the enthropic restoring force stored in the less randomly coiled molecular chains recovered the grating structure, and the molecular chains then reverted to a randomly coiled state; however, this process occurred at a very slow pace due to intermolecular friction. UV irradiation can increase the molecular vibration so that the impacts of the surrounding molecules can accelerate the randomizing process of the molecular chains, therefore recovering the grating structure at a much faster pace.
Fig. 2(b and c) show the absorption and Fourier transform infrared (FTIR) spectra, respectively, of the SPC-347 UV glue before UV curing (termed “glue”), the UV-cured SPC-347 UVCR (termed “resin”), the cured SPC-347 UVCR after heating treatment (120 °C for 30 seconds) (termed “heated resin”), and the heated SPC-347 UVCR after UV irradiation (100 mW cm−2 for 3 hours) (termed “recovered resin”). Clearly, chemical changes are not involved in the morphological change of the SPC-347 UVCR grating, as the spectra of the “resin”, the “heated resin” and the “recovered resin” are highly consistent for both absorption and FTIR. The spectral difference occurs only during the initial UV curing stage (from a glue to a cured resin), as the absorption peak is blue-shifted from 298 nm to 293 nm and the intensity of the CC peaks at 809 cm−1 decreases due to polymerization40 (since π-bonds were involved in the polymerization process). Thus, the morphological change in the grating after the heating treatment is not due to the change in cross-linking since the heating treatment does not jeopardize the cross-linking. Instead, the absorption and FTIR results further confirm that the morphological change in the grating during heating and its structural recovery are, in essence, attributed to pure physical changes.
To investigate the performance of synaptic potentiation and depression, our device was heated repetitively at 120 °C for 10 seconds with an interval of 5 minutes. The interval duration is set to dissipate the heat of the sample, and natural restoration has a minimum effect on the device due the short interval. As shown in Fig. 3(a), the relative diffraction efficiency continues to decrease at a gradually slowing pace, and it eventually becomes steady. An ascending curve with a slow-down pace covering a much longer time scale was observed when the device was treated with UV irradiation repetitively at a dose of 100 mW cm−2 for 30 minutes with an interval of 5 minutes (Fig. 3(b)). These results indicate that similar to a biological engram, our artificial engram device shows an accumulation effect during the memorizing and forgetting processes, and these processes obey the law of diminishing marginal utility and, consequently, arrive at a stable state.
To further investigate the temperature effect, Fig. 3(c) shows the relative diffraction efficiency as a function of both temperature and the number of heating stimuli. For each heating stimulus, our device was heated repetitively at the target temperature for 10 seconds with an interval of 5 minutes. The results show a distinctive stimulus threshold between 70 °C and 80 °C for the heating process, which matches the dividing point of the glassy transition region and the rubbery plateau region in Fig. 2(a). The relative diffraction efficiency of our device exhibits a strong and nonlinear dependence on the temperature and number of stimuli. A higher temperature and a larger number of stimuli lead to a more pronounced change in synaptic weight.
Our device has a property similar to the Ebbinghaus forgetting curve; that is, a timely review will strengthen its memory. Such behaviour was analysed by measurement of paired-pulse facilitation (PPF), a typical form of short-term potentiation caused by two closely occurring stimuli on the time scale. Here, a PPF index, which refers to the changing ratio of two adjacent relative diffraction efficiencies to the initial value, was calculated, and the dependence of the PPF index on the intermediate time Δt between two stimuli is shown in Fig. 3(d). The heating conditions were 100 °C for 10 seconds, and UV irradiation was applied during the intermediate time. The inset shows that the change in the relative diffraction efficiency increases from A1 (under the first stimulus) to A2 (under the second stimulus after Δt), and the PPF index equals the value of A2/A1. The longer Δt is, the lower the PPF index is, which means that a delayed stimulus has a reduced effect, corresponding to a less effective memorizing result when the time gap between the initial learning and subsequent review processes is large.
Furthermore, our device shows a reasonable cycling capability, which enables repetitive use. Fig. 3(e) shows the relative diffraction efficiencies under the cycling test. For each heating period, the temperature was set to 120 °C and the duration was 30 seconds. For each UV irradiation period, the dose was set to 100 mW cm−2 and the duration was 3 hours. The set duration for the restoration process was sufficient so that the sample was fully recovered after each UV irradiation period. The efficiency at the end point of each heating period is consistent (approximately 7 ± 2%), as is the efficiency at the end point of each UV irradiation period (approximately 51 ± 4%), clearly showing the repeatability of the sample under the same stimulus. Although Fig. 3(e) only shows 8 cycling operations, the maximum cycle number that our device can theoretically withstand is expected to be more than tens of thousands, based on the typical fatigue properties of acrylates and fluorine-containing polymers (SPC-347 UVCR is a fluorine-containing acrylate).
According to all the above-mentioned findings, our device meets all the requirements for artificial engrams, including synaptic plasticity, long memory storage time, asymmetric memorizing–forgetting behaviour and measurable changes and responses, and thus can realize the fundamental functions of a biological engram for information encoding, storage and retrieval.
From the biological perspective, an engram is only the first step of memory formation. Strictly speaking, an engram is not yet a memory but rather provides the necessary physical conditions for a memory to emerge.24,41 In 2015, Tonegawa et al. proposed normative usage of engram-related terminologies, including engrams, engram cells, engram cell ensembles, and engram complexes.20,24 The concepts of these biological terminologies and the functions they have can be properly reflected in our artificial engram device, as illustrated in Fig. 3(f). An engram refers to the enduring physical and/or chemical changes triggered by learning, which is reflected by the topological changes of our device under external stimuli. An engram cell is the carrier of a given engram and can support more than one memory. An engram cell is the basic unit of an engram system, so for our device, the minimal stimulable area is defined as a pixel, which corresponds to an engram cell. An important property of engram is that engram cells can support more than one experience, and the co-allocated engram cells make the two experiences linked or integrated. This is due to the nature of competition for allocation to an engram. Within a given brain region, engram cells that have been allocated to a previous experience are more likely to be co-allocated to the engram which supports the memory of a related experience that occurs close in time.24 The engram cells localized within a brain region work together and form an engram cell ensemble, and ensembles in different brain regions, i.e., the hippocampus and amygdala, can be further connected via engram cell pathways and form an engram complex that supports a memory stored in the ensembles distributed throughout the brain. The working mechanism of memory storage and retrieval is based on the specific connectivity patterns of engram ensembles at different brain regions. For our device, a certain combination of activated pixels constitutes a pattern that corresponds to a particular ensemble. Different patterns can overlap and form a pattern complex, mimicking the structure and functions of an engram complex. In the following sections, we will demonstrate how our artificial engram device realizes the functions of engram complexes and fulfils the targets of engram complex-involving memory formation and retrieval, memory manipulation and implantation, and memory consolidation.
The artificially defined pattern is the first step of operations related to memory, which are designed to quickly reflect a contextual identity. For example, patterns A, B, C, etc. can be regarded as engram cell ensembles in the hippocampus that are related to different contexts, including places, light, odours, etc. (termed the conditioned stimulus), and patterns I, II, III, etc. can be regarded as engram cell ensembles in the amygdala that are related to different feelings and emotions (termed the unconditioned stimulus). The topological change of the grating in the patterns is determined by the heating strength, and thus, the overlapping area of the patterns has an enhanced effect. The overlapping degree depends on the connectivity of two events. For example, if a conditioned stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus are strongly correlated, then pattern A and pattern I have a large overlapping area, and a memory linking the context and the feeling is established.
Here, we compare memory formation and retrieval in a biological engram system and an artificial engram system. In the biological system (Fig. 4(a)), when a mouse was exposed to context A, neurons with higher intrinsic excitability transformed into engram cells (the green solid spheres in Fig. 4(a)), and the collection of these cells formed a hippocampal engram ensemble containing contextual information A. If, at the same time, a footshock stimulus was applied, then an amygdala engram ensemble (the group of yellow solid spheres in Fig. 4(a)) was formed and carried valence information I, which represents the fear emotion. Since the context and footshock stimulus were highly correlated in time, the connectivity between the hippocampal engram ensemble and the amygdala engram ensemble was high (as illustrated by the orange dotted lines in Fig. 4(a)). As a result, when the hippocampal engram ensemble was optogenetically activated by light (Fig. 4(b)), not only the context information A but also the valence information I would be read out so that the mouse would freeze as it recalled the experience of being footshocked.20
The same process can be realized in the artificial engram system, and the concept is illustrated as follows. As shown in Fig. 4(c), the system consists of an artificial engram device on a motor-controlled translational stage, a multi-wavelength switchable light source with an adjustable aperture, and a movable photodetector placed at the -1st order diffracted reflection angle. The light source can selectively emit infrared irradiation for heating, UV irradiation for active restoration, and visible light for diffraction efficiency measurement. The aperture is mounted at the outlet of the light source and used to define the shape and area of the pattern. The translational stage can move the device in a plane so that the pattern can be allocated to a target area of the device using (x, y) coordinates. If a conditioned stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus are highly correlated, then the translational stage will allocate close (x, y) coordinates to the pattern of contextual information (i.e., pattern A) and the pattern of valence information (i.e., pattern I), so that the two patterns largely overlap. The photodetector records the light intensity and can distinguish the values from different patterns by moving itself to the -1st order diffracted reflection angle of the target pattern. Although we only demonstrate the practical fabrication of an artificial engram device instead of the entire system, the setup of such artificial engram system is theoretically feasible. For memory formation, contextual information A triggered the formation of pattern A using infrared irradiation (Fig. 4(c)), and highly correlated valence information I led to the formation of pattern I under infrared irradiation with slightly different (x, y) coordinates assisted by the translational stage (Fig. 4(d)). If the two events were not correlated, then the two patterns (i.e., pattern A and pattern II) had no overlapping area (Fig. 4(e)). When context A was recalled (Fig. 4(f)), the visible light for the measurement of diffractive efficiency was incident on pattern A, and the contextual information in pattern A and any correlated valence information could be read out by measuring the change in the relative diffraction efficiency of corresponding patterns. The relative diffraction efficiency η of the patterns was determined using the following equation:
η = I/I0 × A1 + A2, | (1) |
Researchers have also discovered that the memory valence switch is feasible in engrams.20 Here, two unconditioned stimuli (valences I and II) and one conditioned stimulus (context A) are in play, as shown in Fig. 5(c). With the aid of a protein synthesis inhibitor, the former connection between context A and valence I was weakened, and at the same time, the new connection between context A and valence II was built up, resulting in a manipulated valence switch from one to the other (e.g., from fear to pleasure) for context A. In 2019, Vetere et al. paired a genetically specific olfactory glomerulus with optogenetic stimulation of either appetitive or aversive neural pathways, and after this memory implantation process, mice reacted to the real odour that activated this olfactory glomerulus in a predicted manner.42 Such memory valence switching and implantation can also be realized in artificial engram systems. Here, UV irradiation works as a protein synthesis inhibitor and accelerates the weakening of the former connection through its “active forgetting” function (from several days to 3 hours). As shown in Fig. 5(d), the topological change of the grating in the overlapping area of patterns A and I was weakened by UV irradiation and, in the overlapping area of patterns A and II, was strengthened by infrared irradiation. Thus, when light is incident on pattern A, the manipulated valence information in pattern II (pleasure instead of fear) can be retrieved successfully.
To mimic the same function of memory consolidation, two identical artificial engram devices were prepared, one for the HPC-MEC-BLA complex and the other for the PFC ensemble, as shown in Fig. 6(b). The HPC-MEC pattern and PFC pattern had the same pattern shape and position relative to their own device. Each time the valence information of pattern BLA was successfully retrieved when pattern HPC-MEC was illuminated by visible light, pattern PFC on the other device was treated with infrared irradiation. Over time, the topological change in the PFC pattern was strengthened due to repetitive cycles, corresponding to the maturation of the PFC engram ensemble. After a certain time, due to the natural restoring effect of the HPC-MEC-BLA device, the BLA response was no longer able to be activated by illuminating the HPC-MEC pattern with visible light. In contrast, when pattern PFC was illuminated by visible light, the valence information I in the virtual pattern BLA in the right-hand side device (the black dotted outline in Fig. 6(b), which had the same pattern shape and position relative to its own device compared with that in the left-hand side device) can be successfully retrieved. Thus, from this point on, a memory consolidation was established in the PFC, a place in the brain mediating higher-order behaviours, such as intellectual or executive functions, and only the recall of the PFC pattern could activate the BLA response.
Footnote |
† Electronic supplementary information (ESI) available. See DOI: 10.1039/d1nh00064k |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2021 |