Tung S.
Nguyen
,
Julia Y. K.
Chan
*,
Jade T. K.
Ha
,
Ugo
Umekwe-Odudu
and
Sachel M.
Villafañe
Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry, California State University Fullerton, Fullerton, CA, USA. E-mail: juliachan@fullerton.edu
First published on 22nd January 2024
Retention and underrepresentation of diverse ethnic groups have been and continue to be problematic in the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) disciplines in the United States. One foundational course that is required for all STEM majors is general chemistry. One way to increase retention and diversity in STEM majors is by targeting students’ social-psychological beliefs about their academic success through the implementation of social-psychological interventions. These short impactful exercises aim to change students’ thoughts, feelings, and beliefs about their academic success and affective characteristics. In this exploratory study, we designed and implemented two chemistry specific growth-mindset modules (GMMs) in two first-year chemistry courses (general chemistry 1 (GC1) and general chemistry 2 (GC2)) at a Hispanic-Serving Institution (HSI). Students worked on the GMMs asynchronously at two specific time points throughout the semester. Using a mixed-methods approach, we assessed students’: (i) changes in mindset beliefs, chemistry self-efficacy (CSE), and chemistry performance, (ii) perceptions towards failures and challenges, and (iii) perceptions on growth-mindset modules (GMM) after participation in GMMs. Overall, GC2 students shifted towards a growth mindset and away from a fixed mindset, with small to medium effect sizes detected. No statistically significant changes in GC1 students’ mindsets were detected throughout the study period. For both courses, students increased in CSE by the end of semester. Furthermore, GC1 students who participated in any portion of the GMM intervention achieved higher scores on the ACS exam compared to those who didn’t participate. Additionally, students’ written responses highlighted an improved attitudinal change towards failures and challenges after participating in GMMs. For both courses, over 95% of the students agreed that the GMMs were valuable, over 95% students indicated they developed more positive attitudes and perspectives towards challenges, and over 96% students believed they could learn challenging topics with effort, determination, and persistence. While these results show differences in performance, CSE, mindset scores, and attitudinal change after participation in GMMs, it is also important to acknowledge that self-selection into the study may be one of the factors for explaining such differences. Results and implications for practice are discussed.
One tool that has been gaining momentum over the years is implementation of social-psychological interventions in the classroom, particularly growth mindset interventions. These interventions target students’ subjective experiences and have the potential to change their thoughts, feelings, and beliefs in their academic performance (Yeager and Walton, 2011). Some studies have shown these interventions to be more impactful for marginalized groups (Fink et al., 2018).
Very few mindset intervention studies have been conducted in STEM courses (Canning et al., 2019; Campbell et al., 2021), particularly involving chemistry students at the university level (Fink et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2021). In a first-year general chemistry course, Fink et al. (2018) implemented a chemistry specific growth-mindset intervention and showed that the racial achievement gap (marginalized vs. non-marginalized groups) was eliminated. Likewise, Wang et al. (2021) showed that students’ academic performance improved after participating in a growth-mindset intervention in first-year general chemistry course. To date, three other mindset studies in chemistry have been conducted, but none of them included an intervention (Limeri et al., 2020a; 2020b; Santos et al., 2022). In second-semester OC class, Limeri et al. (2020) studied students’ mindsets and found that they became less growth and more fixed over the semester. In particular, those who struggled in the class experienced the most drastic shift towards a fixed mindset and attributed the negative learning experiences as a factor of this change in mindset. In contrast, those who passed the class and overcame challenges attributed their abilities as changeable. Students also associated their prior learning experiences to their current mindset beliefs. Together, these studies indicate the changeability of mindset beliefs and the association between academic performance and mindset beliefs. This emphasizes the importance of enhancing students’ mindsets through timely interventions.
However, in the aforementioned mindset studies, the student demographic makeup is very different from the current study's institution. Given the scarcity of mindset intervention studies in chemistry, particularly in Hispanic-Serving Institutions (HSI), we sought to design and implement chemistry-specific growth-mindset modules (GMMs) in two first-year chemistry courses (first-semester general chemistry (GC1) and second-semester general chemistry (GC2)) and explore students’ mindsets and perceptions towards challenges and failures, and overall perceptions on GMMs. Using a mixed-methods approach, the following research questions were pursued:
1. How does participation in chemistry GMMs influence students’ mindsets, chemistry self-efficacy (CSE), and course performance in chemistry?
2. How does participation in chemistry GMMs affect students’ perceptions towards challenges and failures?
a. To what extent are there differences in students’ perceptions towards challenges and failures between students from marginalized groups compared to students from non-marginalized groups?
3. What are students’ overall perceptions of chemistry GMMs?
In the current study, the later version of DMI was used (Dweck, 2006). Two modifications were made to the original DMI. Firstly, each statement on the DMI was modified by adding the word “chemistry” to specify that chemistry intelligence was specifically what was being measured and to avoid response process validity concerns (Appendix 1). Dweck (2000) found that students’ mindsets can vary across academic disciplines and compared to general beliefs about intelligence, a mindset scale consisting of items with discipline-specific beliefs is more accurate and stronger predictor of students’ learning behaviors and achievement (Shively and Ryan, 2013; Gunderson et al., 2017; Costa and Faria, 2018). Another modification that was made to the DMI in this study was the addition of a direct-response item, which read, “This is a control question. Please select “Disagree 5”, as your response to this question.” This question served to screen out participants who responded to survey questions with insufficient effort (DeSimone et al., 2015). Therefore, the modified DMI implemented in our study consists of nine statements (Appendix 1). Students completed the modified DMI at three time points in the study: week 1 (pre-GMMs), week 3 (post-GMMs) and week 13 (delayed post-GMMs) (Fig. 1).
In addition to participating in two GMMs, all students also participated in a learning strategies (LS) workshop designed by the corresponding author (Fig. 1). The design and implementation of the LS workshop is described elsewhere (Nguyen, 2022). This paper only focuses on the design and initial pilot study results from the implementation of GMMs.
Students in GC1 met either twice a week for 165-minutes each or three times a week for 110-minutes. GC2 students met twice a week for 75-minutes each. All instructors are full-time and tenured or tenure-track, three of which conduct research in chemistry education. Across the two different courses and sections, the instructors met regularly to ensure that the curriculum, class content, and all examinations covered similar material. For GC2, it was taught by one instructor in fall 2021 (N = 83 students) and another instructor in spring 2022 (N = 87 students). For GC1, it was taught by three different instructors in spring 2022 (N = 172) and four different instructors in fall 2022, (N = 151 students). There were two of the same GC1 instructors who taught in both spring 2022 and fall 2022.
For all surveys in this study, student responses were collected online using Qualtrics survey software. Cases with missing data on any of the survey questions were excluded from the analyses. Students’ responses covered the entire range of scales on the surveys. No ceiling or floor effects were observed.
The final number of student participants in the study was calculated by filtering out those who: (i) failed to consent to participate, (ii) did not meet surveys’ deadlines, (iii) participated in a previous implementation of the exact same GMMs in the past (i.e., double exposure to the treatment), and (iv) incorrectly answered the control question on the DMI. Therefore, in GC2, this number was 29 (35% of enrolled) in fall 2021, and 32 (37% enrolled) in spring 2022. In GC1, the final number of students was 34 (20% enrolled) in spring 2022 and 50 (33% enrolled) in fall 2022. To increase statistical power, we combined data across semesters for each course. The implication of these numbers being a fraction of the total population is addressed in Limitations section of this paper.
Students were reminded through e-mail, Canvas, and in lectures of the survey opportunities throughout the semester. As an incentive for completing the surveys, students earned participation points (ranging from 3–5%) toward their course grade. Students who consented to participate and those who did not both earned extra credit by completing the survey. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board. Human subject consent was obtained from all participants before the start of the study.
In the second GMM, students viewed short 10–15 minutes pre-recorded semi-structured interviews featuring a small, yet diverse group of students and professor representing various age groups, sex, ethnicity, and cultural backgrounds from the Department of Chemistry and Biochemistry of the corresponding author's institution. This group included a second-year graduate chemistry education student (male, non-URM), third-year undergraduate chemistry major student (female, non-URM), chemistry alumni (male, URM), and an assistant professor in chemistry (male, URM). During these one-on-one interviews, we asked each of them to share stories of how they overcame failures and challenges in the context of teaching and learning chemistry. The purpose was to intervene early and have GC1 and GC2 students learn the importance of cultivating a growth mindset in the face of challenges encountered when learning chemistry through watching these pre-recorded interviews early in the semester before they encounter challenges.
To assess the effectiveness of the modules, students completed surveys immediately before, immediately after, and ten weeks after participating in GMMs to evaluate how their beliefs about intelligence towards chemistry change throughout the semester. Additionally, students were asked to provide feedback for future modules and reflect on how participation in GMMs have influenced their attitude, perception, and motivation toward learning challenges (Appendix 3). Engaging in deliberate reflections where students are asked to connect key concepts learned from the intervention through writing exercises serves to strengthen one's belief in it. This practice, also known as “saying is believing” effect, is common in social-psychological interventions (Higgins and Rholes, 1978; Van den Hurk et al., 2019).
For the written reflection questions, all students’ responses were coded, and a summary of the themes and codes were identified using the constant comparison method (Glaser and Strauss, 1999). Responses were coded by two undergraduate students, two graduate chemistry education students, and the corresponding author. The research group is made up of a diverse team of members representing different ethnic backgrounds, sex, academic background, and age groups. After training and several discussions, an interrater agreement of 88% (for the second research question) and 96% (for the third research question) was reached among all raters. More details will be discussed in the results section under each of these research questions.
Data-model fit was assessed using indices such as Comparative Fit Index (CFI) (Bentler, 1990), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger, 2016), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) (Chen, 2007). CFA conducted showed CFI ranging from 0.888 to 0.915, RMSEA 0.090 to 0.108 and SRMR 0.047 to 0.053 (Appendix 4). According to Hu and Bentler (1999), the acceptable ranges were of CFI values greater than 0.95, RMSEA values less than 0.06, and SRMR values less than 0.08. However, according to McNeish et al. (2018), acceptable CFI values could be as low as 0.775 and RMSEA values as high as 0.20. Naibert et al. (2021) corroborated these values in their CFA. Based on these findings, acceptable data-model fits were assessed using the following cutoff values: CFI (greater than 0.775), RMSEA (less than 0.20), and SRMR (less than 0.08). Because the produced CFA indices fell within the acceptable reported ranges, they confirmed that the DMI measures two constructs (fixed and growth mindsets) (Limeri et al., 2020). Appendices 5, 6, and 7 show the CFA models for three time points, weeks 1, 3, and 13, respectively. The sample sizes used to conduct the CFAs in weeks 1, 3, and 13 were N = 377, N = 339, and N = 346, respectively.
To validate the CSE instrument, the same procedure and range of fit indices (CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR) were used as described above. The analyses found CFI to range from 0.953 to 0.996, RMSEA 0.033 to 0.109 and SRMR 0.020 to 0.032 (Appendix 4). CFA results confirmed a single construct was being measured (CSE) using the instrument (Ferrell and Barbera, 2015). Appendices 8 and 9 show the CFA models for two time points, weeks 1 and 13, respectively. The sample sizes used to conduct the CFAs in weeks 1 and 13 were N = 231 and N = 275, respectively.
Participate in at least one GMM | N | Z-ACS exam score | SD | t | df | Effect size (d) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note: ACS exam scores are z-scored. *** p < 0.001. | ||||||
Yes | 216 | 0.15 | 0.98 | 3.76*** | 326 | 0.44 |
No | 112 | −0.28 | 0.99 |
Participate in both GMMs | N | Z-ACS exam score | SD | t | df | Effect size (d) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note: ACS exam scores are z-scored. *p < 0.05. | ||||||
Yes | 79 | 0.23 | 0.86 | 2.39* | 326 | 0.32 |
No | 249 | −0.07 | 1.03 |
Although this study did not test for the causal effect of growth mindset interventions on academic achievement, our findings suggest that the performance improvement may be associated with participation in GMMs. Specifically, students who participated in any portion of the GMM intervention achieved higher on the ACS exam compared to those who didn’t participate. Previous research studies have shown that growth mindset interventions have varying levels of predictive power on achievement measures such as improved grades (Blackwell et al., 2007; Broda et al., 2018, Fink et al., 2018) through adaptive behaviors learned from the intervention (Hong et al., 1999). However, it is important to acknowledge that self-selection into the study may be one of the factors explaining the differences in ACS exam scores. It is unknown whether students who self-selected to participate in the study were inherently more motivated to succeed in the course compared to non-study participants. Thus, it is important to consider the potential impact of self-selection bias as it could influence the interpretation of results.
GC2 students started off with a higher mean CSE score (M = 3.34, SD = 0.57) compared to GC1 students (M = 2.95, SD = 0.59). However, the magnitude of change in CSE throughout the semester was smaller for GC2 compared to GC1. For the GC1 population, a very large effect size was detected (d = 0.97) compared to GC2 population (d = 0.34) (Appendix 11). Similar to our findings, Moreno et al. (2021) found that students enrolled in general chemistry started the semester with a higher chemistry self-efficacy compared to students enrolled in introductory chemistry, but students in introductory chemistry experienced a greater change of CSE after a semester of instruction. However, it is important to acknowledge that self-selection into the study may be one of the factors explaining the differences in CSE scores. This will be further elaborated in the limitations of the manuscript.
To assess how students’ mindsets changed over the study period, a 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA test was conducted with mindset scores (fixed score, growth score) as the dependent variable and time as the within-subjects (independent) variable (weeks, 1, 3, & 13). Overall, there were no statistically significant changes in GC1 students’ fixed and growth mindsets throughout the three time points (Fig. 3 and 4). In contrast, there were statistically significant changes in GC2 students’ fixed and growth mindsets (Fig. 3 and 4) with small to medium effect sizes detected. Specifically, GC2 students’ fixed mindset scores increased (less fixed) from week 1 to 3 (Fig. 3) and GC2 students’ growth mindset scores decreased (more growth) from weeks 1 to 3 and weeks 1 to 13 (Fig. 4). Below is a summary breakdown for each study population.
The overall trends in mindset scores showed that students shifted towards a stronger growth mindset and a less fixed mindset from weeks 1 to 3 and from weeks 1 to 13 for both GC1 and GC2 students. Interestingly, from weeks 3 to 13, this trend (stronger growth mindset, less fixed mindset) continued for GC2 students. However, for GC1 students during this same time period, the reverse trend was observed (stronger fixed mindset, less growth mindset). During this ten-week period, no additional GMMs were implemented. Students took multiple assessments within the ten weeks and depending on their performance, this may lead them to change their beliefs about their intelligence about chemistry (Limeri et al., 2020b). These shifts in mindset beliefs across the three time points suggest the instability of mindset beliefs and a need to consistently remind students about growth mindset throughout the semester. It is important to note that these are general trends observed, but not all trends were tested to be statistically significant (i.e., GC1) due to low sample sizes and hence, these trends should be interpreted with caution. In contrast to our findings, Limeri et al. (2020b) found that second-semester organic chemistry students became less growth and more fixed by the end of semester and this trajectory of mindset change was related to their experience with academic challenges. This difference in mindset trends may be explained by the very different demographic makeup of the study population in each research study.
It is interesting to note that although the sample size for GC2 was smaller (N = 40) than GC1 (N = 60), statistical significance and small to medium effect sizes were detected (d = 0.29–0.48). One possible reason for why statistical significance was detected in GC2 (and not GC1) may be due to differences in instructor effects. In GC2, there were only two instructors teaching the course; for GC1, there were five different instructors teaching the course at the time of data collection. Since all students participated in GMMs asynchronously, instructor variability across the different sections should not be a concern. However, we did not have control over how much emphasis and reinforcement of growth mindset discussions instructors had in each classroom. Research has shown that instructor's mindsets have a direct influence on students’ mindset and their academic performance (Canning et al., 2019). Whether faculty members adopt a growth or fixed mindset will affect the way they communicate and advise students, the tone and attitude they choose to use, and how they encourage or discourage students to keep trying in the face of challenges. The increased number of different instructors teaching GC1 compared to GC2 may have contributed to an increased variability in students’ responses thereby making it more difficult to detect a statistically significant difference in students’ mindsets (i.e., high noise to low signal ratio).
Additionally, it is also interesting to note that GC2 students started with a more fixed mindset compared to GC1 students. A possible reason for why GC2 students started off with a worse mindset than GC1 students may be influenced by their negative academic experiences encountered from prior chemistry courses. This is important to address because whether students view their abilities as a fixed or growth trait has significant implications for their responses to failure and academic outcomes. Limeri et al. (2020b) found that second-semester organic chemistry students experienced worsening of mindset over the course of a semester (i.e. shift towards a fixed mindset and away from a growth mindset) with many mentioning prior negative academic experiences to be a factor. Together, these findings imply that mindset beliefs are dynamic and can be influenced by prior students’ experiences.
Theme | Description |
---|---|
Acceptance | Indications of development of positive outlook/mindset on failures and challenges (viewing them as opportunity or using them as motivation/encouragement) |
Indications of accepting failures and challenges as part of learning and a willingness to persist and overcome challenges. | |
Essential characteristics | Indications of students realizing the key characteristics needed to overcome challenges (e.g., having self-belief, determination, self-assurance, hard work, persistence, and strategic practice) |
Behavioral | Indications of planning/implementing new approaches/actions when encountering failures and challenges. |
Neutral | Indications of unchanged perception/attitude |
“I have become more accepting towards failure and challenges, and just see them as a means to be optimistic and positive towards myself and the situation”. (non-marginalized group, GC1)
“I have learned that failure is also an important process that is needed in order to learn and better myself. Seeing this I have begun to see failure and challenge as things that are necessary for my growth.” (non-marginalized group, GC2)
Furthermore, for both GC courses, students from the non-marginalized group revealed they were more likely to welcome failures and challenges as part of the learning experience compared to their counterparts (60% non-marginalized group (GC1); 61% non-marginalized group (GC2)).
“It has made me realize that as long as I have determination and persistence in something that [I’m] trying to learn, I will be able to succeed.” (marginalized group, GC1)
“Always believe in yourself.” (marginalized group, GC2)
When disaggregating the data by minority status, there was no difference in frequency of comments between students from marginalized group vs. non-marginalized group in GC1. However, in GC2, more students from non-marginalized group recognized the necessity of having certain essential characteristics to overcome challenges (62% non-marginalized group, GC2).
“I keep trying and ask for help when needed.” (marginalized group, GC1)
“Definitely change my perspective in how I need to study in order to pass university classes. I need to put in the time in order to successfully learn and pass all my courses.” (non-marginalized group, GC2)
In GC1, there were no differences in frequency of students’ comments between marginalized vs. non-marginalized groups. However, in GC2, more students from the non-marginalized group realized the importance of modifying their study behaviors after participating in GMMs (58% non-marginalized group, GC2).
“It has not really. I still am pretty focused when I need to be.” (marginalized group, GC1)
“My mindset about failure and challenges have not changed that much because I have always had the mindset that challenges are good learning opportunities and failure is a requirement to succeed.” (non-marginalized group, GC2)
In both courses, GMMs had a stronger impact on transforming the attitudes and perceptions of marginalized students towards challenges and failures when compared to non-marginalized group students (33% marginalized groups, GC1; 19% marginalized group, GC2).
Overall, students from the non-marginalized group were more accepting of failures and challenges and their comments indicated they were more able to recognize the importance of reflecting, adapting, and modifying their study behaviors when they encounter challenges and failures. In addition, students from the non-marginalized group were better at recognizing the importance of having specific traits for overcoming challenges. While students from the non-marginalized group showed higher awareness of these factors, the GMMs had a less pronounced impact on them. Instead, students from the marginalized group were more impacted by the GMMs, particularly in terms of transforming their attitudes and perceptions towards challenges. Research has shown that students with higher self-efficacy are more likely to persist and display positive affect when going through challenging tasks and exercise more self-regulated behaviors compared to students with lower self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977; Zimmerman, 2000; Komarraju and Nadler, 2013). The frequency of students’ comments suggests that non-marginalized students may have higher self-efficacy compared to marginalized students. In a semester of a college-level preparatory chemistry course, Villafañe et al. (2016) showed that Black and Hispanic males started the semester with a higher initial chemistry self-efficacy. However, as the semester progressed, their chemistry self-efficacy decreased compared to White males (Villafañe et al., 2014). This finding suggests that chemistry self-efficacy is influenced by students’ prior experiences and different subgroups of students experience the course in different ways throughout the semester. Consequently, these different experiences impact students’ beliefs about their ability to overcome challenges, the way in which they approach and respond to challenges in their coursework.
The themes that emerged from this study corroborated with mindset-related constructs found in prior research studies on growth mindset (Dai and Cromley, 2014; Smiley et al., 2016; Fink et al., 2018) and highlight different stages of the cognition-affect-behavior chain (Dweck and Leggett, 1988). The cognition-affect-behavior chain reflects the different stages of mindset development and how one's beliefs about intelligence influences their emotions and actions. It is important to note that many students described their ability to transition from thinking negatively about themselves to accepting mistakes and persevering during challenging experiences after attending the modules. This transition led to a positive outlook on failures and the belief that success comes from hard work as explained by a student below:
I always had a fixed mindset. I thought I was a failure and will never succeed in life. Instead of challenging myself, I just give up and start to think negatively about myself. However, after participating in these[modules], I’m beginning to think that maybe I just need to be more patient, and if I work hard enough, I will ultimately reach my full potential. To grow as a person, I need to start challenging myself and realize that it's okay to make mistakes and fail. (GC2, non-marginalized)
Triangulating students’ mindsets scores with their written responses revealed development of mindset-related constructs such as accepting challenges and failures as part of the learning process, gaining new perspectives, adopting new approaches towards studying, and developing an overall positive attitude towards failure and challenges (Table 3).
After the growth mindset sessions, I believe I can learn challenging topics with effort, determination, and persistence | I believe the growth mindset sessions were valuable | After the growth mindset sessions, when I encounter challenging situations, I now approach them with a more positive attitude and perspective | |
---|---|---|---|
Strongly disagree | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Disagree | 1 (0.9%) | 1 (0.9%) | 1 (0.9%) |
Somewhat disagree | 3 (2.7%) | 2 (1.8%) | 3 (2.7%) |
Somewhat agree | 12 (10.9%) | 13 (11.8%) | 18 (16.3%) |
Agree | 40 (36.3%) | 41 (37.2%) | 46 (41.8%) |
Strongly agree | 54 (49%) | 53 (48.2%) | 42 (38.2%) |
Total responses | 110 |
After the growth mindset sessions, I believe I can learn challenging topics with effort, determination, and persistence | I believe the growth mindset sessions were valuable | After the growth mindset sessions, when I encounter challenging situations, I now approach them with a more positive attitude and perspective | |
---|---|---|---|
Strongly disagree | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) |
Disagree | 0 (0%) | 1 (1.4%) | 1 (1.4%) |
Somewhat disagree | 1 (1.4%) | 2 (2.8%) | 2 (2.8%) |
Somewhat agree | 6 (8.5%) | 13 (18.3%) | 15 (21.1%) |
Agree | 34 (47.9%) | 23 (32.4%) | 26 (36.7%) |
Strongly agree | 30 (42.3%) | 32 (45%) | 27 (38%) |
Total responses | 71 |
Theme | Sub-theme | Description |
---|---|---|
Areas of strengths | Resources | Students appreciate articles, videos, and outside resources provided. |
Development of new perspectives | Students gain positive experiences, perspectives, mindsets, and ideas | |
Content | Students appreciate the type of information presented and the structure workshop | |
Areas of improvement | Interaction | Students request for more interactive activities. |
Modification | Students request for modifying content of modules by adding more information about growth-mindset and/or specific assignments that help them practice growth mindset | |
Neutral | No suggestions or improvements needed |
I think strengths in the [modules] were the videos since they were entertaining and had a lot of information. (GC2)
Some of the strengths were the video and the reflection questions […] (GC1)
Furthermore, students valued the written reflective questions throughout the modules as the questions promoted deep internalization of the concept of a growth mindset and the ability to immediately apply the concept within their individual contexts. Reflective writing, a common and recognized social-psychological intervention, has been well documented to be effective in reinforcing one's beliefs (i.e. “saying is believing” effect) (Higgins and Rholes, 1978). In addition, 34.5% GC1 and 30.7% GC2 students reported having more positive outlooks on failures and challenges as an area of strength garnered from after attending the GMMs. It is worthy to note that students could highlight this new perspective towards challenges and failure without the help of a prompt. In the second research question, we specifically prompted students to describe their perspectives towards challenges and failures after participating in GMMs, and the themes “acceptance” and “essential characteristics” emerged (Table 3). These themes corroborated with “development of new affective perspectives” described within this section (Table 6). For example, some students commented on the following:
I really like how encouraging these [modules] were. Shows me that no matter what, I can accomplish anything if I tried […] (GC2)
Some strengths are helping others see that growth mindsets are essential to improving. (GC1)
24.1% GC1 and 41.1% GC2 students also mentioned the content presented as another strength of the GMMs. For example,
I also thought that the real life videos of people from different stages of life being able to share their experiences of failures, allows me to understand that we are not perfect and that we are bound to make mistakes and that is okay. (GC1)
I think that the open ended questions about what the students have personally struggled with are really good because they leave a lot of room for interpretation of different experiences. (GC1)
This student appreciated the personal anecdotes and stories shared by current students and professor. Learning from real people and hearing about their personal experiences were more impactful and resonated with the students more than being taught theories or concepts about growth mindsets.
An improvement I suggest for future [modules] is to try to have more interactive activities to test our knowledge of growth mindsets. (GC2)
I would suggest more examples on how you could change your mindset, as well as the benefits, other than the anatomical benefits. (GC1)
These students’ comments highlighted the necessity of converting asynchronous modules into interactive in-person workshops and integrating additional intentional interactive activities to help students put what they learn into practice, particularly in the asynchronous environment to build more engagement. For example, instructors can design small reflection assignments that utilizes growth-mindset related strategies before and after each exam to actively remind students about the importance of having growth-mindset during challenging times throughout the semester. Furthermore, some students were interested in learning more about practical ways to shift their mindsets, understanding the benefits of cultivating a growth mindset, and delving into a more in-depth understanding of the connection between growth mindset and neuroplasticity.
In my opinion, I don’t think there are any improvements that should happen for future [modules] because I believe it was amazing and helped me really think [for] the first time. (GC2)
I did not really see any points of weakness that would need improvements. (GC1)
Overall, using a mixed-methods study design which involved a pre- post-, and delayed-post data collection process, the effect of GMMs on student learning could be more holistically studied. Quantitatively, statistical analysis revealed no statistically significant differences in mindset scores among GC1 students across the study period. However, statistically significant increases in growth mindset and decreases in fixed mindset were detected for GC2 students. While quantitative findings did not distinctly show the effectiveness of the GMMs for the two courses, students’ written responses provided much more in-depth insights into the effectiveness of GMMs.
Using a mixed-methods study design which involved a pre-, post-, and delayed-post (weeks 1, 3, 13) data collection process, we examined: (i) students’ mindset beliefs, chemistry self-efficacy, and chemistry performance, (ii) perspectives towards challenges and failures, and (iii) overall perceptions of the novel GMMs.
Overall, there were no statistically significant changes in GC1 students’ mindsets throughout the study period. In contrast, there were statistically significant changes in GC2 students’ mindsets with small to medium effect sizes detected. GC2 students became more growth in mindset from weeks 1 to 3 and from weeks 1 to 13 and became less fixed in mindset from week 1 to 3. For both GC1 and GC2, there were statistically significant increases in students’ chemistry self-efficacy by the end of semester after participating in GMMs. Furthermore, our analysis showed that students who participated in any portion of the GMM intervention achieved higher on the ACS exam compared to those who didn’t participate. While results show that ACS exam scores, CSE, scores, and mindset scores improved after participation in GMMs, it is also important to acknowledge that self-selection into the study may be one of the factors for explaining the differences in these scores detected as it is unknown whether motivation may be a confounding variable. Further discussion is provided in the limitations section of this manuscript.
Students’ written responses highlighted an improved and new perspective towards viewing failures and challenges. Students from non-marginalized group showed a higher degree of: (i) acceptance of failures and challenges (acceptance), (ii) belief in their abilities to succeed and overcome challenges (essential characteristics), and (iii) active planning or finding strategies to conquer challenges and failures (behavioral). The higher frequency of students’ comments in these categories suggests that non-marginalized students may have higher self-efficacy compared to marginalized students, show a higher tendency to persist through challenges, and demonstrate a greater ability to cope with challenges.
Furthermore, students appreciated the resources shared, content delivered, and new perspectives gained as key strengths of the GMMs. Some students requested inclusion of more interactive activities and additional content to supplement the existing content in GMMs as areas of improvements. Overall, for both courses, students recognized the strength, value, and utility of the GMMs. Over 95% of students agreed that the GMMs were valuable, over 95% students indicated they developed more positive attitudes and perspectives towards challenges, and over 96% students believed they could learn challenging topics with effort, determination, and persistence in chemistry.
Together, the quantitative and qualitative results from this exploratory study highlight the potential benefits of chemistry specific GMMs implemented in two first-year chemistry courses. However, given the low sample size in our current study, it's important to acknowledge the data presented may not entirely represent the characteristics of the target population. Further discussion is provided in the limitations section of this manuscript.
Another way instructors can provide students with opportunities to encounter challenges and practice a growth mindset is by integrating course-based undergraduate research experience (CUREs) into the lab setting. In CUREs, students engage in challenge-based learning by performing authentic research that is intrinsically fraught with setbacks. Gin et al. (2018) showed that a CURE explicitly designed to embed failure and practice into the laboratory resulted in students’ increased ability to go through scientific challenges compared to students who participated in CUREs that did not embed failures. In addition, students who failed developed more resilience and understanding that setbacks during the learning experience were normal and should be viewed as learning opportunities instead of “failures”.
Many student comments highlighted a key strength of the GMMs was learning from real people and hearing about how they overcame personal academic challenges through watching the pre-recorded interview videos. Students’ comments revealed they felt “validated”, “less alone”, and “can easily relate” when they heard about the academic challenges that students and professor went through. Limeri et al. (2020b) also found that students’ beliefs about intelligence were influenced by observing and hearing stories of their peers overcoming academic challenges and recognizing that it was possible for them to overcome them as well. Therefore, future mindset interventions could emphasize on this social element by having peers share personal stories of how they overcame challenges and encourage students to view the ability to overcome challenges in learning chemistry through a growth-oriented approach that encompasses developing the correct type of study strategies, consulting the right resources, and practicing strategic effort.
Additionally, several student comments highlighted they would like to learn more about the integration of neuroplasticity, cognition, and nature of science in addition to what is already covered in the GMMs. Limeri et al. (2020b) found that another factor that influenced students’ beliefs about intelligence was reasoning from scientific principles, such as teaching students about brain plasticity. To effectively engage the college-level student audience who may have a more sophisticated knowledge of brain plasticity, it may be necessary to include more details about the neurobiology of learning and emphasize on the physical changes that occur in the brain for future mindset interventions.
Finally, instructors’ beliefs about their students’ intelligence and ability are equally important as it plays a key role in students’ academic achievement. When students believe that their instructors believe in their ability to learn and overcome challenges (i.e. have a growth mindset), they are more inspired to perform and achieve more (Canning et al., 2019; LaCosse et al., 2020; Muenks et al., 2020). This implies how and what we choose to communicate to students in the classroom is highly important as it shapes their motivation and achievement. If instructors can create more opportunities to cultivate growth mindset cultures in their classes, this could potentially inspire more students to pursue STEM fields.
Second, it is important to note that the results reported herewith in are limited to a single HSI in the United States and cannot be generalizable to other institutions and settings. The data collected in this study were from a diverse sample of students across several sections and semesters of general chemistry lecture courses. This diverse sample, representing a wide spectrum of perspectives and experiences, can provide additional insights into the views of the target population.
Third, it is important to acknowledge the limitation of self-reported data used in surveys, such as social desirability and acquiescence bias. Whether students provided responses in which they truly developed and held throughout the study period is unknown. Future work can include follow-up interviews with students to further probe their views on mindset beliefs which may provide more insight on such limitations.
Fourth, it's important to acknowledge that self-selection bias could affect the interpretation of results. Prior to the intervention, study participants and non-study participants were not compared on incoming metrics, such as self-efficacy, motivation, or prior knowledge. Therefore, it is unknown whether students who self-selected to participate in the study had similar background characteristics compared to non-study participants. To assess if the two groups are equivalent or not, a prior knowledge test and or survey can be distributed prior to the intervention. Additionally, future mindset interventions can consider using a randomized controlled trial (RCT) study design to test the true effect of the intervention by minimizing the impact of confounding variables (e.g., student motivation).
Fifth, students participated in GMMs early in the semester before taking any high-stakes assessments. The purpose of having students participate in GMMs in the first three weeks of the semester was to intervene early and have students learn the importance of cultivating a growth mindset before they encounter challenges, such as taking high-stakes exams. Therefore, most of the student survey data and reflection data collected occurred before exam 1. It is important to note if the GMMs were implemented later in the semester, after exams, the results reported herewith in may differ.
Finally, we acknowledge the limitations of using the term “chemistry intelligence” to replace “intelligence” in the modified DMI. While Costa and Faria (2018), Gunderson et al. (2017), and Shively and Ryan (2013) found that the mindset scale is more accurate and predictive of academic outcomes when items are context-specific to the course being studied, Santos et al. (2021) and Limeri et al. (2020a) found that modifying mindset instrument with the addition of the discipline name, “chemistry intelligence” led to a more growth-mindset centered distribution compared to other STEM-specific studies. These findings imply that students interpret general intelligence and chemistry intelligence in a broad range of ways, which may lead to potential response process concerns. One way to improve measurements on chemistry intelligence is to incorporate more specific terms which align with common perceptions of “intelligence” and “ability” in chemistry. These terms include “knowledge”, “ability to apply”, and “understanding” on certain chemistry-related tasks (Santos et al., 2021). Future work can include collecting mindset data using the CheMI instrument and comparing it to mindset data collected using the modified DMI instrument to identify if there are any differences in the way students are interpreting “chemistry intelligence”.
Footnote |
† Electronic supplementary information (ESI) available. See DOI: https://doi.org/10.1039/d3rp00352c |
This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2024 |